ETH Zurich's weekly web journal - auf deutsch
ETH Life - wissen was laeuft ETH Life - wissen was laeuft


ETH Life - wissen was laeuft ETH Life - wissen was laeuft
Home

ETH - Eidgenoessische Technische Hochschule Zuerich - Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich
Section: Campus Life
deutsche Version english Version
Print-Version Drucken

Published: 29.04.2004, 06:00
Modified: 28.04.2004, 17:51
How ETH deals with scientific misconduct
Research off the rails

Where there are people, there is cheating. This is also true in the scientific community. ETH responds to this fact with the introduction of various measures, amongst others new rules of procedure on scientific conduc have been issuedt. An ETH sociologist shares his views on scientific fraud and its consequences.

By Christoph Meier

The ability to deceive is a sign of intelligence. Only human beings are capable of deliberate deception to any great degree, although it takes them a couple of years to acquire this capability. There is only anecdotal evidence that other animals can deceive with intention. Naturally, cheating is despised within the scientific community – generally held to be the palladium of intelligence – and in no way is it considered to be a glorious intellectual deed.

Triggered by the Schön affair

ETH is also concerned about the integrity of its research. One reason for the present uneasiness is the controversy surrounding the scientific fraud affair that was exposed in 2002 involving the physicist, Jan Hendrik Schön, in the areas of microelectronics and superconductivity. One of the co-authors of many of the articles that contained faked data is a physicist who, in the meantime, has joined ETH. Professor of Physics Bertram Batlogg headed the team that included Schön. Although an inquiry carried out by an independent scientific committee exonerated the ETH scientist and judged he had reacted appropriately and met his responsibility as co-author, the question also arose as to whether a distinguished head of research like Batlogg had adopted a critical enough attitude to the research under investigation.

For the ETH Direction the affair was reason enough to set up a working group in 2003, which included Nobel laureate Richard Ernst, to formulate guidelines for research carried out at ETH Zurich and regulations on procedures in cases of scientific misconduct. In the meantime the ETH Executive Boeard has ratified the new "Rules of Procedure Governing Allegations of Scientific Misconduct at ETH Zurich" that comes into force on 1st May 2004 (1). A further measure is that all who carry out research at ETH have received copies of the paper "The Research Culture at ETH Zurich“ and the brochure "On being a scientist“. On top of this, last autumn the Collegium Helveticum at ETH launched a series of events under the title of "Debating Science Culture" (2), to encourage scientists to undertake a process of critical self-assessment. The series runs until the end of this semester.

Loss of confidence a major problem

"Scientific Frauds" is the title of the next event on 26th April, in which Bertram Batlogg and Andreas Diekmann will also take part. Diekmann is ETH Professor of Sociology and caused quite a stir with his inaugural lecture last semester entitled "Scientific fraud and deception". In his lecture the sociologist named loss of trust as the most serious consequence of scientific fraud – both within and outside the world of research. When asked for an examples from the history of science to illustrate this point Diekmann first thinks of Friedhelm Herrmann. The fraud practised by this German cancer researcher, which became public in 1997, had led to a storm of outcries from scientific bodies and the media.

Where does fraud begin?

But weren't Schön and Herrmann simply two isolated cases, exceptions to the rules that govern scientific research that couldn't seriously damage science as a whole? Diekmann is sceptical. First of all, because every new case of fraud leads to a loss of trust. "If an airline loses three planes in crashes in a single year, passengers won't book with this airline, anymore, even though they know that any plane belonging to any airline might crash." The sociologist says we also have to be cautious because the frequency of scientific misconduct is an unknown quantity. This has to do with the fact that scientific transgressions are often more difficult to identify than, for example, traffic violations. In addition there is a grey area where things are not so clear-cut that one can speak of fraud. "Take, for instance, the analysis of statistical data. Test x is not significant while test y delivers a result with the desired significance. So the researcher writes up on the results of test y," explains Diekmann. This is leading to a situation where the "honest" manuscript containing lots of "ifs" and "buts" stands less chance of being published in the journals.

Controlling practice insufficient

Despite grey areas and the difficulties of identifying scientific wrongdoings Diekmann thinks the new ETH rules of procedure on scientific misconduct is an improvement on the present situation. "Even though we haven't got a radar trap or a Breathalyser for fraud there is nevertheless a need to set out 'a highway code' for research." Experience has shown that the current practice of self-regulation isn't sufficient. New institutional regulation is therefore a prudent step, says Diekmann. He would also welcome it if spot-checks were carried out to check the validity of data and ensure that proper scientific methodology has been used, especially in areas where duplication is not likely to happen. There is no reason why this couldn't be done within the framework of a scientific self-control. Because he doesn't believe that scientists are a better category of human being.


continuemehr

The specialist media also reacted strongly to the fraudulent publications of the physicist Jan Hendrik Schön. large

Diekmann says this doesn't mean that one can do without scientific morals or training practice for honest working methods. "Most scientists carry out punctilious work, even though deception could bring them material advantages or be beneficial to their careers."

He himself approaches the issue of fraud by seeking out methods that can show indications of the falsification of specific data and statistics. Under certain conditions a so-called Benford distribution proves suitable to find indications of anomalies. Naturally, such a procedure can never provide particular details of a forger, like a fingerprint.

What level of trust does this ETH scientist himself place in his colleagues after all the time he has spent looking into forgeries? Diekmann says that he gives everyone he meets or works with a sort of "trust in advance", and adds, "But when I read of new results in a scientific publication, I first believe that these have been independently duplicated." For those readers who think that the careful attitude of this scientist is not enough, Mark Twain comes to the rescue. He proposed the following method to get at the truth: "There is only one way to ascertain whether a man is honest – ask him. If he says 'yes' then one knows that he is dishonest."


"Forgers, tricksters, charlatans"

(mib) The mathematician Charles Babbage was probably the first scientist to look into the issue of fraud in a systematic manner. It was later taken up by a number of writers, most recently by Heinrich Zankl in "Forgers, tricksters and charlatans" (Fälscher, Schwindler, Scharlatane) (Wiley-VCH). Here some examples of famous scientists who have been accused of fraud.

- Albert Einstein carried out experiments in 1915 together with Johannes de Haas on the magnetic properties of iron. Following calculations using the widely accepted gyromagnetic factor at that time, the result should have been a factor of 1. After two series the researchers arrived at 1.45 and 1.02, respectively. But Einstein and de Haas only published the second factor. Later on it transpired that 2 is the right factor.

- Galileo Galilei didn't even go to the trouble of testing his hypothesis of "sloped planes" in an experiment. As this natural scientist drew conclusions from this for the increased velocity of free-falling bodies, it is more than questionable whether he checked this assumption. Another scientist who worked from his desk is Bruno Bettelheim. This citizen of Vienna counts as one of the most important child and youth psychologists of the 20th century. But: this tenured professor at the University of Chicago had never studied psychology nor did he develop his "environment therapy“ from his own experiences.

- Plagiarism? This is the question the Nobel Foundation had to ask itself in 1972 when the Italian physicist Oreste Piccioni demanded compensation. The reason? He had devised the experiment that was carried out by Emilio Segré and Owen Chamberlain and which led to the discovery of the antiproton. Segré and Chamberlain were awarded the Physics Nobel Prize for this. There was also something amiss in the award of Nobel Prize in 1923 – but not until 70 years later did it transpire that Robert A. Millikan had been less than accurate in his determination of the charge of the electron.

- The results presented by Martin Fleischmann and Stanley Pons from the University of Utah were almost too good to be true. In the spring of 1989 this pair of chemists claimed that they had succeeded in finding an electro-chemical method to fuse the atom. The trouble was than nobody was able to duplicate their results. Victor Ninov from the Lawrence Berkeley Laboratory also discovered something that only he was able to see: chemical elements nos. 116 and 118. It later transpired that Ninov had forged data.

- A further discovery that belongs in the realm of fairy stories is that of Jacques Benveniste concerning the proof for the effects of homeopathy. Despite the fact that the results, published in "Nature", still crop up time and again, IgE antibodies do not exchange information with water molecules – even in very high potentiations. Equally erroneous are Trofim Desinowitsch Lyssenko's wheat growth trials, the crystalline images of t-RNA from Hasko Paradies, and Friedrich F. Friedmann's vaccination study on tuberculosis.




Footnotes:
(1) "Regulations Governing Allegations of Scientific Misconduct at ETH Zurich" (in German): www.rechtssammlung.ethz.ch/pdf/415_fehlverhalten_forschung.pdf
(2) Debating Science Culture: www.kontrovers.ethz.ch/



You can write a feedback to this article or read the existing comments.




!!! Dieses Dokument stammt aus dem ETH Web-Archiv und wird nicht mehr gepflegt !!!
!!! This document is stored in the ETH Web archive and is no longer maintained !!!